National Family Literacy 2003 in Long Beach!

Jan 2003

Profile:
Rhona Genzel


Prospects for 2003

Andrews Text Reviewed

VanPatten Text Reviewed

/ Index /
/ Letters /
/ Search /

Subscribe
for free!


ESL MiniConference Online!

LAD Isn't Just A Black Box Any More
A Review of Bill VanPatten's Newest Text

New Book by Bill VanPatten The first copies of Bill VanPatten's new text, From Input to Output: A Teacher's Guide to Second Language Acquisition (McGraw-Hill), were churned out at the plant in the final days of 2002 and began arriving via U.S. mail on January 2nd, 2003. This 114-page book is written with the purpose of making current SLA theory "comprehensible" to teachers in training as well as non-academic readers. I was able to finish reading the book in about seven hours, over the course of several sittings, but then again I spent a lot of time reflecting on the 40 different "Pause to Consider" sections which Dr. VanPatten has inserted at crucial junctures in the text.

From Input to Output has the potential to impact a new generation of ESL/EFL practitioners as deeply as Stephen Krashen and Tracy Terrell's The Natural Approach influenced those of us starting our ESL careers when it appeared in 1983. Krashen and Terrell's siren song indirectly contributed to the development of a breach between ESL teaching and formal linguistics; VanPatten has effectively reunited linguistic theory and language teaching by explaining what SLA researchers now understand about the once vague notion of "comprehensible input."

First, I'd like to point out three editing errors in the text. On page 53, about midway through the second paragraph, "participle" is misspelled. On page 72, in the last complete paragraph, the sentence "The latter is an effortful process" should have used either "former" or "effortless" in order to make sense. On page 96, in the parenthetical remark about midway through the second complete paragraph, the main verb "be" was inadvertently omitted.

VanPatten also does not quite successfully achieve a clear explanation of what syntactic "parameter resetting" is. He could have borrowed analagous terminology from the field of electronics in order to make his meaning clearer, or he could have incorporated the phrase "fixed range" and utilized more insights from the 1995 volume of the Cornell Working Papers, where "Input and Parameter Resetting in Second Language Acquisition," by Theresa Antes, Christine Moritz, and Regina Roebuck, appeared.

Bill VanPatten has, nonetheless, written a pivotal text which new as well as experienced ESL teachers are going to use to inform their choices of approaches, methods and materials. The chapter devoted to input itself is one of the most eloquent explanations you will find anywhere. VanPatten's main point is that input which is useful for language acquisition must be communicative, which means the purpose is communication and the focus is on meaning. Another key concept explained by VanPatten is "intake," which is the amount of input the learner can process within his or her "working memory." This chapter will be suggestive of many implications for how ESL and other language teachers craft the tasks which students must perform.

Throughout this insightful new textbook, VanPatten continually reiterates his strong belief that learning a language is different in several important ways from general learning of knowledge. For one thing, he says, most other learning is done through the use of language. "Language acquisition is different from any other kind of learning," says VanPatten. "A belief that most theorists and researchers in SLA share is that learning a second language involves processes and parts of the brain that are different from those used to learn other things." He goes on to ask, "If language is unique to humans, is it possible that the brain has evolved in a way that treats language separately from other human cognitive abilities?"

The central chapter in From Input to Output is about how language learners develop an implicit system of lexical, syntactic and pragmatic networks which is continuously adapting new elements and restructuring itself as it responds to new comprehensible input. According to VanPatten, explicit knowledge about a new language "can play only a supporting or ancillary role," perhaps helping learners to process input better. He compares explicit knowledge to crutches for someone with a broken leg. "In SLA, explicit knowledge may be used as a 'crutch' under certain conditions while the processes used to develop a rule in the implicit system have their chance to work," VanPatten explains.

In the chapter on output, VanPatten describes the mechanisms involved when a language learner unconsciously accesses his or her implicit system of networks (or more deliberately accesses explicit knowledge about the new language) in order to engage in communication using the L2. "Access" is defined as "activating the lexical items and grammatical forms necessary to express particular meanings." The second half of output, according to VanPatten, are "production strategies," which tend to be based on the learner's L1 early in his or her language learning experience and on the developing L2 system as language acquisition progresses.

VanPatten also touches on a relatively new area of SLA research, regarding the potential role of the learner's output in the language acquisition process. He does not believe that output plays the same sort of role as input in the second language acquisition process. However, he does think output can make a different indirectly because of the way it is involved in "negotiation of meaning," prompting another participant in the communication event to contribute input which adjusts syntax or otherwise clarifies meaning. "Making output may push [learners] to be better processors of input, something they might not do otherwise," explains VanPatten. "In this case, output may be necessary for continued growth."

The design of Bill VanPatten's new book seems well-suited to a variety of teacher-education learning styles. He follows his explication of comprehensible input, the implicit L2 system, and output strategie, with a chapter of FAQs (Frequently Asked Questions). Here are the questions which VanPatten answers thoughtfully and clearly, and in the process underscores the main themes of the entire text.

Does First Language Cause Interference?
What About the Use of the First Language in the Classroom?
Is SLA Like First Language Acquisition?
Don't Imitation and Repetition Play a Role in Acquisition?
So Drills Don't Help, Then?
Do Learners Develop Bad Habits If They Aren't Corrected?
Doesn't Giving Learners Rules Help? (That's the Way I Learned.)
So Does Teaching Make No Difference?
Are Some Languages More Difficult to Learn Than Others?
What Makes Some Structures Difficult and Some Easy to Acquire?
Doesn't Everything in SLA Come Down to Motivation?
Does Age Make a Difference in Language Learning?
Don't Different Levels of Learners Need Different Things to Help Them Keep Learning?
What About Individual Differences? Should I Consider Learning Styles?
What's the Best Method for Teaching and Learning?

The last chapter in From Input to Output: A Teacher's Guide to Second Language Acquisition gives some of Dr. VanPatten's own suggestions for how to approach the language teaching challenge based on current models of second language acquisition. Here are the five implications he draws:

1. The More Input, the Better (The More Meaning-Based the Class, the Better)
2. The More Interaction, the Better
3. All Learner Production Should Be Meaning-Based, or Communicative
4. Focus on Form (or Grammar Instruction) Should Be Meaning-Based and Tied to Input or Communication
5. We Should Watch Out for What We Expect of Learners

In this final chapter, VanPatten gives some great examples of ways to make our classroom lessons more communicative, which means having to do with the negotiation of meaning. He warns against "eclectic" approaches which are not based on clear principles informed by what is known about second language acquisition processes. He reiterates what may be considered his main thrust throughout the book: the best chance a language teacher has of enhancing L2 acquisition is in making certain his or her classroom provides an environment rich in the kind of meaningful input which promotes the growth of a learner's implicit (unconscious) system of L2 networks.

Bill VanPatten's argument might remind the reader of the allegory of the cave in Book 7 of Plato's The Republic, where Socrates suggests that "certain professors of education must be wrong when they say that they can put a knowledge into the soul which was not there before," and that the best a teacher can do is turn his or her student's attention in the direction of "the brightest and best of being, or in other words, of the good" (P.F. Collier & Son, 1901, translated by Benjamin Jowett).

VanPatten's new text constitutes a major effort to reorient the focus of language teachers (and language learners) towards classroom interactions which provide the crucial ingredients needed for efficient L2 acquisition. It is a thoroughly accessible book and a great starting point for further debates about unconscious and conscious aspects of language learning. Bill VanPatten has reopened the channels (or floodgates?) of communication between ESL teachers and linguists with this most recent contribution. As you read this textbook, maybe, like me, you will find your own system of intuitions about language learning enriched, as your network of teaching principles restructures itself to adapt to the terms and concepts of current SLA research.

Story by Robb Scott

2003 ESL MiniConference Online